Elsevier

Journal of Biomechanics

Volume 147, January 2023, 111438
Journal of Biomechanics

Characterization of trial duration in traditional and emerging postural control measures

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2023.111438Get rights and content

Abstract

Researchers may select from varied technological and practical options when evaluating balance. Methodological choices inform the quantitative outcomes observed and allow practitioners to diagnose balance abnormalities. Past investigations have differed widely on sampling duration, and these discrepancies hinder comparisons among studies and confidence in outcomes where trials were excessively short. This study aimed to identify necessary trial lengths for common and emerging center of pressure-based measures. We hypothesized that dependent variables would fluctuate over time but eventually reach a stable magnitude. Ninety-seven apparently healthy adults performed quiet standing for 180-seconds (s) with eyes (A) open and (B) closed on a force platform. Anterior-posterior and medial–lateral elements of the center of pressure were used to calculate velocity, time-to-boundary, and Hurst exponents using 15, 30, 90, 120, 150, and 180 s of data. Two-way repeated measures ANOVAs were used to differentiate postural measures over time and between vision conditions. Outcomes were considered stable when significant changes in the measure were no longer observed in the time factor. Dependent measures stabilized for velocity between 60 and 120 s, time-to-boundary between 120 and 150 s, and the Hurst exponent between 30 and 120 s. Velocity measures stabilized quicker with eyes open, whereas vision had no effect or the eyes closed condition was faster to stabilize in time-to-boundary and detrended fluctuation analysis measures. We conclude that 150 s of standing data is sufficient to capture a broad range of postural stability outcomes regardless of vision condition.

Introduction

Maintaining stable posture or balance is a fundamental task that underlies the activities of daily living, largely contributing to the well-being, independence, healthspan, and lifespan of an adult human being. Static posturography has become an integral part of the balance assessment process, allowing researchers and clinicians to detect balance abnormalities and differentiate between populations (Blaszczyk, 2016, Prieto et al., 1996, Roman-Liu, 2018). Traditionally, quiet (i.e., static) standing is completed by tracking the center of mass or, more commonly, the center of pressure (CoP) over time. Force plates, force platforms, or balance plates derive the CoP coordinates, enabling the extraction of descriptive measures (Richmond et al., 2021a). These derived measures may be confounded by several methodological practices, including sample frequency (Rhea et al., 2015), stance (i.e., foot) positioning (Cobb et al., 2014, Kirby et al., 1987, Uimonen et al., 1992), noise (Park et al., 2011), and visual feedback (D'Anna et al., 2017). Experimental data and tradition have produced standard technological (e.g., sample frequency, duration, filtering) and participant-focused (vision, stance) static balance protocols for research and clinical practice. However, optimal trial duration is far from a settled debate.

The length of a standing trial is one suggested key regulatory mechanism of postural control (van der Kooij et al., 2011), influencing the reliability and validity of descriptive measures during quiet standing (Carpenter et al., 2001, Le Clair and Riach, 1996). For example, CoP velocity may require 60-seconds (s) of standing data for reliable outcomes (Carpenter et al., 2001, van der Kooij et al., 2011). When sample durations are extended beyond 60 s, the probability of incorporating both high and low-frequency components of the CoP signal is enhanced, as opposed to the increased prominence of higher frequencies during shorter sampling durations, indicating that an extension of the sample duration to at least 180-seconds is required for a reliable assessment of visual implications on quiet standing (van der Kooij et al., 2011) However, this recommended sample duration was based on outcomes from a limited sample size (n = 10) (van der Kooij et al., 2011) and further work seems warranted to investigate this question.

Prior examinations regarding trial duration in standing tasks have also prioritized comprehensive (e.g., position, velocity, or frequency) descriptive measures and do not advise on sample durations for advanced discrete (i.e., time-to-boundary (TtB)) static postural measures. TtB incorporates the instantaneous position and movement of the CoP relative to the base of support (Richmond et al., 2021a), representing the time window available to make corrective postural changes in the prevention of a fall (Hertel et al., 2006, Richmond et al., 2020, Slobounov et al., 1998, van Wegen et al., 2002), offering an alternative conceptual view of postural control and higher sensitivity compared to comprehensive measures (Whittier et al., 2020). Currently, there is little consensus on the approach to capture TtB. Prior TtB investigations have evaluated 10 s (Cobb et al., 2014, Hertel et al., 2006, Hertel and Olmsted-Kramer, 2007, McKeon et al., 2010, McKeon and Hertel, 2007), between 10 and 20 s (at least 10 s of data cropped) (Yamanaka et al., 2012), 20 s (Richmond et al., 2020), 30 s (D'Anna et al., 2015, DiLiberto et al., 2021, Richmond et al., 2021b, Wikstrom et al., 2010), and 60 s (D'Anna et al., 2017) of data. Several of these evaluations utilize multiple trials and produce aggregate outcomes from the repeated trials (Cobb et al., 2014, D'Anna et al., 2015, Hertel et al., 2006, Hertel and Olmsted-Kramer, 2007, Wikstrom et al., 2010, Yamanaka et al., 2012). However, averaging across multiple shorter trials (e.g., ten trials of 60-seconds) to produce aggregate outcomes do not overcome the limitations of measuring shorter compared to longer trial durations (van der Kooij et al., 2011), and to date, no work has established a minimum trial length for obtaining reliable TtB outcomes.

Finally, detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA) can be used to estimate the Hurst exponent of the increment process (velocity) for fractional Brownian motion (position) (Peng et al., 1995). DFA allows the sample to be broken down into segments (e.g., 1–5 s) to identify a scaling relation between the residual errors of the trendline and the time window of the sample (Delignières et al., 2011). Prior work utilizing this popular approach has recorded standing data for at least 20 s (Amoud et al., 2007), 30 s (Hansen et al., 2017), 65 s (Wang and Yang, 2012), or as long as 75 s (Kim et al., 2008). (van der Kooij et al., 2011) concluded that 60 s of data is appropriate to capture DFA measures. They further speculate that the DFA coefficients should be relatively insensitive to duration as long as the length of the longest time window used is still short relative to the trial duration (van der Kooij et al., 2011). The variance in these measures is inversely related to sample duration, as the underlying process is sampled over a greater number of data segments. (van der Kooij et al., 2011) suggests that the increased variance for shorter duration samples, specifically those under 180 s, may cause finding statistically significant effects of sample duration more difficult. There is room for additional exploration on this family of outcomes given that study’s limited sample size, as noted above.

Common and emerging CoP-based measures either lack experimental support for trial duration or, where recommendations are given, limited sample size restricts their generalizability. The overarching aim of this work was to advance the understanding of postural control by evaluating trial duration’s effects on CoP-based postural deficiency metrics in neurotypical adults. We hypothesized that dependent measures would either increase or decrease over time but trend toward a value that would remain stable for the rest of the trial. Implications of this research could affect the clinical assessment and diagnosis of balance impairments, in addition to the evaluation of rehabilitation approaches.

Section snippets

Participants and the role of the funding source

We recruited a convenience sample of ninety-seven (68 males and 29 females) neurotypical adults (demographic and anthropometric summaries; Table 1). Eligible participants were free of: (1) any neurological impairments, (2) injuries that would impact balance over the previous year (e.g., concussions, musculoskeletal injuries, etc.), and (3) medications that would influence postural stability. All participants self-reported activity via the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) and

Results

All dependent postural variables displayed large changes throughout the trial, regardless of vision condition (see Table 2, Table 3).

Except for path length, all measures reached a stable magnitude at 150 s or sooner (Fig. 1A and Fig. 1B). For CoP velocity, significant interactions were observed in each represented measure, and in subsequent contrasts, it was determined that a minimum of 60 s is necessary to stabilize AP velocity with eyes open but 90 s with eyes closed (Fig. 1D). Alternatively,

Discussion

This investigation further establishes our perception of postural control by evaluating the effects of sampling duration on traditional and emerging CoP-based postural deficiency metrics in neurotypical adults. Our hypothesis that key posturography variables would fluctuate early and then plateau during sustained quiet standing was accepted. This work builds on prior work from (van der Kooij et al., 2011) and comments on an open question regarding postural stability sampling durations by

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Luigi Auriemma and Maurice (Paul) Franzese for their assistance in acquiring data for this project. This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

References (52)

  • C.K. Rhea et al.

    Interpretation of postural control may change due to data processing techniques

    Gait Posture

    (2015)
  • J.T.E. Richardson

    Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research

    Educ. Res. Rev.

    (2011)
  • S.B. Richmond et al.

    The assessment of center of mass and center of pressure during quiet stance: Current applications and future directions

    J. Biomech.

    (2021)
  • S.B. Richmond et al.

    Advanced characterization of static postural control dysfunction in persons with multiple sclerosis and associated neural mechanisms

    Gait Posture

    (2021)
  • D. Roman-Liu

    Age-related changes in the range and velocity of postural sway

    Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr.

    (2018)
  • H. van der Kooij et al.

    Sampling duration effects on centre of pressure descriptive measures

    Gait Posture

    (2011)
  • E.E.H. van Wegen et al.

    Postural orientation: Age-related changes in variability and time-to-boundary

    Hum. Mov. Sci.

    (2002)
  • C.C. Wang et al.

    Using detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA) to analyze whether vibratory insoles enhance balance stability for elderly fallers

    Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr.

    (2012)
  • E.A. Wikstrom et al.

    Postural control differs between those with and without chronic ankle instability

    Gait Posture

    (2010)
  • M. Woollacott et al.

    Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research

    Gait Posture

    (2002)
  • H. Amoud et al.

    Fractal time series analysis of postural stability in elderly and control subjects

    J. Neuroeng. Rehabil.

    (2007)
  • S.C. Cobb et al.

    The relationship among foot posture, core and lower extremity muscle function, and postural stability

    J. Athl. Train.

    (2014)
  • D'Anna, C., Gazzellini, S., Petrarca, M., Vasco, G., Castelli, E., Schmid, M., Conforto, S., 2015. Time to boundary...
  • C. D'Anna et al.

    Time-to-Boundary Function to Study the Development of Upright Stance Control in Children

    Open Biomed. Eng. J.

    (2017)
  • D. Delignières et al.

    Transition from persistent to anti-persistent correlations in postural sway indicates velocity-based control

    PLoS Comput. Biol.

    (2011)
  • J.E. Donnelly et al.

    American College of Sports Medicine Position Stand. Appropriate physical activity intervention strategies for weight loss and prevention of weight regain for adults

    Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.

    (2009)
  • Cited by (3)

    View full text